Loading…

Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study

This work employs Evoked Potential techniques as 19 participants are confronted with sentences that have the potential to produce scalar implicatures, like in Some elephants have trunks. Such an Underinformative utterance is of interest to pragmatists because it can be considered to have two differe...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Brain and language 2003-05, Vol.85 (2), p.203-210
Main Authors: Noveck, Ira A, Posada, Andres
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a597t-8fe6336574d3ff640dfc6504cacfe44b88580190e7a27e57ae715a8ef71729753
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a597t-8fe6336574d3ff640dfc6504cacfe44b88580190e7a27e57ae715a8ef71729753
container_end_page 210
container_issue 2
container_start_page 203
container_title Brain and language
container_volume 85
creator Noveck, Ira A
Posada, Andres
description This work employs Evoked Potential techniques as 19 participants are confronted with sentences that have the potential to produce scalar implicatures, like in Some elephants have trunks. Such an Underinformative utterance is of interest to pragmatists because it can be considered to have two different truth values. It can be considered true when taken at face value but false if one were to treat Some with the implicature Not All. Two accounts of implicature production are compared. The neo-Gricean approach (e.g., Levinson, 2000) assumes that implicatures intrude automatically on the semantics of a term like Some. Relevance Theory ( Sperber & Wilson, 1985/1996) assumes that implicatures are effortful and not automatic. In this experiment, the participants are presented with 25 Underinformative sentences along with 25 sentences that are Patently True (e.g. Some houses have bricks) and 25 that are Patently False (e.g. Some crows have radios). As reported in an earlier study ( Noveck, 2001), Underinformative sentences prompt strong individual differences. Seven participants here responded true to all (or nearly all) of the Underinformative sentences and the remaining 12 responded false to all (or nearly all) of them. The present study showed that those who responded false to the Underinformative sentences took significantly longer to do so that those who responded true. The ERP data indicate that: (a) the Patently True and Patently False sentences prompt steeper N400’s—indicating greater semantic integration—than the Underinformative sentences and that (b) regardless of one’s ultimate response to the Underinformative sentences, the N400’s were remarkably flat, indicating no particular reaction to these sentences. Collectively, the data are taken to show that implicatures are part of a late-arriving, effort-demanding decision process.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00053-1
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_hal_p</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_00653805v1</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0093934X03000531</els_id><sourcerecordid>73236368</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a597t-8fe6336574d3ff640dfc6504cacfe44b88580190e7a27e57ae715a8ef71729753</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqF0UFPHCEYBmBi2uhW_Qk2XNrUw9iPYRgYL2azaavJJh5qk94IMh9d2plhBWYT--uddTd69EQgD_DlfQk5Y3DBgNVffwI0vGh49fsL8HMAELxgB2TGoIGiZEK8I7MXckQ-pPQXgLFKsUNyxErJRcPVjCwXKxONzRj9fz_8oXmFNPseqQ1jTEiDo2agvl933po8Rryk84HiJvzDlq5DxiF70yWa8tg-npD3btrg6X49Jr--f7tbXBfL2x83i_myMKKRuVAOa85rIauWO1dX0DpbC6issQ6r6l4poYA1gNKUEoU0KJkwCp1ksmyk4MfkfPfuynR6HX1v4qMOxuvr-VJvzwBqwRWIDZvs551dx_AwYsq698li15kBw5i0EkJOcZVvQslLXvNaTVDsoI0hpYjuZQQGetuNfu5Gb4PXwPVzN3o7ycf9B-N9j-3rrX0ZE_i0ByZZ07loBuvTq6uUACbl5K52DqeMNx6jTtbjYLH1EW3WbfBvjPIEpFGpxQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>73236368</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study</title><source>ScienceDirect Freedom Collection 2022-2024</source><source>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</source><creator>Noveck, Ira A ; Posada, Andres</creator><creatorcontrib>Noveck, Ira A ; Posada, Andres</creatorcontrib><description>This work employs Evoked Potential techniques as 19 participants are confronted with sentences that have the potential to produce scalar implicatures, like in Some elephants have trunks. Such an Underinformative utterance is of interest to pragmatists because it can be considered to have two different truth values. It can be considered true when taken at face value but false if one were to treat Some with the implicature Not All. Two accounts of implicature production are compared. The neo-Gricean approach (e.g., Levinson, 2000) assumes that implicatures intrude automatically on the semantics of a term like Some. Relevance Theory ( Sperber &amp; Wilson, 1985/1996) assumes that implicatures are effortful and not automatic. In this experiment, the participants are presented with 25 Underinformative sentences along with 25 sentences that are Patently True (e.g. Some houses have bricks) and 25 that are Patently False (e.g. Some crows have radios). As reported in an earlier study ( Noveck, 2001), Underinformative sentences prompt strong individual differences. Seven participants here responded true to all (or nearly all) of the Underinformative sentences and the remaining 12 responded false to all (or nearly all) of them. The present study showed that those who responded false to the Underinformative sentences took significantly longer to do so that those who responded true. The ERP data indicate that: (a) the Patently True and Patently False sentences prompt steeper N400’s—indicating greater semantic integration—than the Underinformative sentences and that (b) regardless of one’s ultimate response to the Underinformative sentences, the N400’s were remarkably flat, indicating no particular reaction to these sentences. Collectively, the data are taken to show that implicatures are part of a late-arriving, effort-demanding decision process.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0093-934X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1090-2155</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00053-1</identifier><identifier>PMID: 12735938</identifier><identifier>CODEN: BRLGAZ</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>San Diego, CA: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Anatomical correlates of behavior ; Behavioral psychophysiology ; Biological and medical sciences ; Brain - physiology ; Cognitive science ; Electroencephalography ; Electrooculography ; Evoked Potentials - physiology ; Functional Laterality - physiology ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Humans ; Implicature ; Language ; Linguistic-Pragmatics ; Linguistics ; N400 ; Neuroscience ; Psychology ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Reaction Time ; Scalar terms ; Semantics ; Speech Perception</subject><ispartof>Brain and language, 2003-05, Vol.85 (2), p.203-210</ispartof><rights>2003</rights><rights>2003 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a597t-8fe6336574d3ff640dfc6504cacfe44b88580190e7a27e57ae715a8ef71729753</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-a597t-8fe6336574d3ff640dfc6504cacfe44b88580190e7a27e57ae715a8ef71729753</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>230,315,786,790,891,27957,27958,31305</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=14850177$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12735938$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://hal.science/hal-00653805$$DView record in HAL$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Noveck, Ira A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Posada, Andres</creatorcontrib><title>Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study</title><title>Brain and language</title><addtitle>Brain Lang</addtitle><description>This work employs Evoked Potential techniques as 19 participants are confronted with sentences that have the potential to produce scalar implicatures, like in Some elephants have trunks. Such an Underinformative utterance is of interest to pragmatists because it can be considered to have two different truth values. It can be considered true when taken at face value but false if one were to treat Some with the implicature Not All. Two accounts of implicature production are compared. The neo-Gricean approach (e.g., Levinson, 2000) assumes that implicatures intrude automatically on the semantics of a term like Some. Relevance Theory ( Sperber &amp; Wilson, 1985/1996) assumes that implicatures are effortful and not automatic. In this experiment, the participants are presented with 25 Underinformative sentences along with 25 sentences that are Patently True (e.g. Some houses have bricks) and 25 that are Patently False (e.g. Some crows have radios). As reported in an earlier study ( Noveck, 2001), Underinformative sentences prompt strong individual differences. Seven participants here responded true to all (or nearly all) of the Underinformative sentences and the remaining 12 responded false to all (or nearly all) of them. The present study showed that those who responded false to the Underinformative sentences took significantly longer to do so that those who responded true. The ERP data indicate that: (a) the Patently True and Patently False sentences prompt steeper N400’s—indicating greater semantic integration—than the Underinformative sentences and that (b) regardless of one’s ultimate response to the Underinformative sentences, the N400’s were remarkably flat, indicating no particular reaction to these sentences. Collectively, the data are taken to show that implicatures are part of a late-arriving, effort-demanding decision process.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Anatomical correlates of behavior</subject><subject>Behavioral psychophysiology</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>Cognitive science</subject><subject>Electroencephalography</subject><subject>Electrooculography</subject><subject>Evoked Potentials - physiology</subject><subject>Functional Laterality - physiology</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Implicature</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Linguistic-Pragmatics</subject><subject>Linguistics</subject><subject>N400</subject><subject>Neuroscience</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Scalar terms</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Speech Perception</subject><issn>0093-934X</issn><issn>1090-2155</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2003</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7T9</sourceid><recordid>eNqF0UFPHCEYBmBi2uhW_Qk2XNrUw9iPYRgYL2azaavJJh5qk94IMh9d2plhBWYT--uddTd69EQgD_DlfQk5Y3DBgNVffwI0vGh49fsL8HMAELxgB2TGoIGiZEK8I7MXckQ-pPQXgLFKsUNyxErJRcPVjCwXKxONzRj9fz_8oXmFNPseqQ1jTEiDo2agvl933po8Rryk84HiJvzDlq5DxiF70yWa8tg-npD3btrg6X49Jr--f7tbXBfL2x83i_myMKKRuVAOa85rIauWO1dX0DpbC6issQ6r6l4poYA1gNKUEoU0KJkwCp1ksmyk4MfkfPfuynR6HX1v4qMOxuvr-VJvzwBqwRWIDZvs551dx_AwYsq698li15kBw5i0EkJOcZVvQslLXvNaTVDsoI0hpYjuZQQGetuNfu5Gb4PXwPVzN3o7ycf9B-N9j-3rrX0ZE_i0ByZZ07loBuvTq6uUACbl5K52DqeMNx6jTtbjYLH1EW3WbfBvjPIEpFGpxQ</recordid><startdate>20030501</startdate><enddate>20030501</enddate><creator>Noveck, Ira A</creator><creator>Posada, Andres</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>8BM</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>1XC</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20030501</creationdate><title>Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study</title><author>Noveck, Ira A ; Posada, Andres</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a597t-8fe6336574d3ff640dfc6504cacfe44b88580190e7a27e57ae715a8ef71729753</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2003</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Anatomical correlates of behavior</topic><topic>Behavioral psychophysiology</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>Cognitive science</topic><topic>Electroencephalography</topic><topic>Electrooculography</topic><topic>Evoked Potentials - physiology</topic><topic>Functional Laterality - physiology</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Implicature</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Linguistic-Pragmatics</topic><topic>Linguistics</topic><topic>N400</topic><topic>Neuroscience</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Scalar terms</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Speech Perception</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Noveck, Ira A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Posada, Andres</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL)</collection><jtitle>Brain and language</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Noveck, Ira A</au><au>Posada, Andres</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study</atitle><jtitle>Brain and language</jtitle><addtitle>Brain Lang</addtitle><date>2003-05-01</date><risdate>2003</risdate><volume>85</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>203</spage><epage>210</epage><pages>203-210</pages><issn>0093-934X</issn><eissn>1090-2155</eissn><coden>BRLGAZ</coden><notes>ObjectType-Article-1</notes><notes>SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1</notes><notes>ObjectType-Feature-2</notes><notes>content type line 23</notes><abstract>This work employs Evoked Potential techniques as 19 participants are confronted with sentences that have the potential to produce scalar implicatures, like in Some elephants have trunks. Such an Underinformative utterance is of interest to pragmatists because it can be considered to have two different truth values. It can be considered true when taken at face value but false if one were to treat Some with the implicature Not All. Two accounts of implicature production are compared. The neo-Gricean approach (e.g., Levinson, 2000) assumes that implicatures intrude automatically on the semantics of a term like Some. Relevance Theory ( Sperber &amp; Wilson, 1985/1996) assumes that implicatures are effortful and not automatic. In this experiment, the participants are presented with 25 Underinformative sentences along with 25 sentences that are Patently True (e.g. Some houses have bricks) and 25 that are Patently False (e.g. Some crows have radios). As reported in an earlier study ( Noveck, 2001), Underinformative sentences prompt strong individual differences. Seven participants here responded true to all (or nearly all) of the Underinformative sentences and the remaining 12 responded false to all (or nearly all) of them. The present study showed that those who responded false to the Underinformative sentences took significantly longer to do so that those who responded true. The ERP data indicate that: (a) the Patently True and Patently False sentences prompt steeper N400’s—indicating greater semantic integration—than the Underinformative sentences and that (b) regardless of one’s ultimate response to the Underinformative sentences, the N400’s were remarkably flat, indicating no particular reaction to these sentences. Collectively, the data are taken to show that implicatures are part of a late-arriving, effort-demanding decision process.</abstract><cop>San Diego, CA</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>12735938</pmid><doi>10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00053-1</doi><tpages>8</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0093-934X
ispartof Brain and language, 2003-05, Vol.85 (2), p.203-210
issn 0093-934X
1090-2155
language eng
recordid cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_00653805v1
source ScienceDirect Freedom Collection 2022-2024; Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
subjects Adolescent
Adult
Anatomical correlates of behavior
Behavioral psychophysiology
Biological and medical sciences
Brain - physiology
Cognitive science
Electroencephalography
Electrooculography
Evoked Potentials - physiology
Functional Laterality - physiology
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Humans
Implicature
Language
Linguistic-Pragmatics
Linguistics
N400
Neuroscience
Psychology
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Reaction Time
Scalar terms
Semantics
Speech Perception
title Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-09-22T09%3A28%3A07IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_hal_p&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Characterizing%20the%20time%20course%20of%20an%20implicature:%20An%20evoked%20potentials%20study&rft.jtitle=Brain%20and%20language&rft.au=Noveck,%20Ira%20A&rft.date=2003-05-01&rft.volume=85&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=203&rft.epage=210&rft.pages=203-210&rft.issn=0093-934X&rft.eissn=1090-2155&rft.coden=BRLGAZ&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00053-1&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_hal_p%3E73236368%3C/proquest_hal_p%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a597t-8fe6336574d3ff640dfc6504cacfe44b88580190e7a27e57ae715a8ef71729753%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=73236368&rft_id=info:pmid/12735938&rfr_iscdi=true